Friday, March 9, 2018

Electoral Support for Donald Trump: Analyzing the Effects of Blind Retrospection and Racial Attitudes on Vote Choice


            In November of 2016, Donald Trump shocked the country by defeating Hillary Clinton in the election for the presidency. The unexpected outcome spurred a debate among academics over how and why a candidate with no political experience, a long list of personal scandals, and a dearth of policy knowledge could have won an electoral contest for the most powerful political office in the country. One theory is that Trump’s supporters were motivated by what Achen Bartels calls blind retrospection (Bartels, 2016). Regardless of the quality of the candidate or the policies of the political parties competing in an election, voters who believe they are suffering from socioeconomic problems in the months before the election become more likely to vote for the challenger over a member of the incumbent party. An alternative theory is that Trump supporters were motivated by his populist rhetoric during the campaign, which tapped into feelings of symbolic racism and even old fashioned racism among America’s white population.  Trump frequently spoke negatively about Blacks, Hispanic immigrants, and Muslims, which may have earned him votes among whites with strong feelings of racial resentment and ethnocentric biases. Which of these theories is a better predictor of vote choice?
            To test these theories, I analyzed data from the 2016 American National Election Survey (ANES), which was conducted following the presidential election and contained a stratified sample of the American population. The dependent variable vote choice is binary as votes for Donald Trump were coded as one and votes for Hillary Clinton were coded as zero.  Votes for the third party candidates were eliminated from the data, which left a total of 1,171 respondents.  Since the dependent variable vote choice is categorical, this requires the use of a binary logistic regression model to determine which independent variables had a greater probability of influencing the outcome of the election.
            To measure the independent variable blind retrospection, I used a question from the 2016 ANES that asked respondents about the state of the economy. They were given the following five choices: 1. Very good 2. Good 3. Neither good nor bad 4. Bad 5. Very bad.  Self-reported feelings on economic status is a better measure of blind retrospection than actual unemployment numbers or some other macro measure of the economy since we are concerned with peoples’ perceptions of reality and not reality itself. It is my hypothesis that respondents who rate themselves higher on this scale will have a greater probability of voting for Donald Trump. Furthermore, Achens’ theory of blind retrospection assumes that voters behave this way due to political ignorance. I use a question on the ANES survey that asked respondents to rate the extent to which they followed politics on a scale of one to five with one being always and five being never. An independent variable measuring the level of education is also included in the model. The second hypothesis is that lower levels of political knowledge, when interacted with perceptions of economic status, will make a voter more likely to vote for Trump due to blind retrospection.
            Measuring the impact of the other independent variable racism was far more challenging as there are several theories on racism in America and how to measure the concept. The first type of racism, old fashioned is racism, is the belief that another race is biologically inferior to one’s own. The political science literature shows that old fashioned racism was prominent in American history up until the Civil Rights Era, then old fashioned racism declined after the 1960’s to the point where the ANES survey stopped measuring it by the 1990’s. However, several political scientists such as Michael Tessler believe that Barrack Obama’s electoral victory in 2008 led to the reemergence of old fashioned racism within the population (Tessler, 2013). Furthermore, many scholars argue that old fashioned racism didn’t completely disappear as respondents on surveys are aware of the social repercussions of answering questions on race honestly. Since the ANES did not directly measure old fashioned racism in 2016, I used the Black Feeling Thermometer to test whether negative attitudes towards blacks made a voter more likely to vote for Donald Trump.  This is an imperfect solution. The third hypothesis is that lower scores on the black feeling thermometer will make voters more likely to vote for Donald Trump.
            The second type of racism, symbolic racism or modern racism, is cultural and not biological in nature. While old fashioned racism declined in the 1960’s, many political scientists argue that a new form of racism emerged to replace it as many whites opposed the redistribution of wealth to black communities, school integration, and affirmative action policies (Sniderman et al., 1991). The majority of conservative whites have blamed problems in the black community on a culture of laziness and entitlement while denying the impact of the legacy of slavery and Jim Crow on present socioeconomic and political inequalities. Numerous studies have shown that symbolic racism is correlated with opposition to social welfare policies (Kinder and Sears, 1981; Desante, 2013) as well as tougher attitudes towards criminal justice (Weizer, 2004).  Symbolic racism has led to the persistence of black stereotypes among whites, and these attitudes have affected white political behavior in terms of voting, party identification, and opinion formation. To measure symbolic racism, I chose five frequently used questions from the ANES survey (Tarman and Sears, 2005):
1.       ‘Generations of slavery and discrimination have created conditions that make it difficult for blacks to work their way out of the lower class.’
2.       Are you for or against preferential hiring and promotion of blacks?
3.       On a scale of one to seven, how much should the government help blacks?
4.       Do you favor, oppose, or neither favor nor oppose allowing universities to increase the number of black students studying at their schools by considering race along with other factors when choosing students?
5.       ‘It’s really a matter of some people not trying hard enough, if blacks would only try harder they could be just as well off as whites.’ Agree/disagree.[1]

 These questions either have ordinal responses on a scale of one to five, ordinal responses on a scale of one to seven, or they only allow the respondent to agree or disagree. With the exception of question five on work ethic, higher scores on these variables indicate more symbolically racist attitudes. My fourth hypothesis is that individuals that score higher on measurements of symbolic racism will be more likely to vote for Donald Trump.
            Finally, ethnic identity should also influence how voters perceive Donald Trump if his racialized rhetoric influenced the outcome of the election. Dummy variables were used for blacks, Hispanics, and Asians while whites were kept as the constant.  My fifth hypothesis is whites should be more likely to vote for Trump than ethnic minorities when controlling for other variables.
            There are also a great variety of other factors that can influence the outcome of an election, so several control variables are needed here. Within the political science literature, party identification is seen as one of the strongest variables that influence voter choice (Druckman, Petersen, and Slothus, 2013). Dummy variables are used for Republican and Democrat, and the constant is used for independents.  Political ideology also influences voter choice. A question is used from the 2016 ANES survey that asks respondents to rate whether they are liberal or conservative on a scale of one to seven with seven being the most conservative.  A standard set of demographic variables are also used in the logit model, including age, gender, income, education, the importance of religion, and marital status. A question that measures attitudes towards authoritarianism is also included.  
            Two logistical regression models were conducted, one of which produces coefficients and another which produces the odds ratio for each variable. The results are as follows:
Trump Vote




Coefficients
Robust Standard Errors





Republican
1.58
(0.35)
***
Democrat
-2.23
(0.35)
***
Ideology
0.56
(0.13)
***
Female
-0.19
(0.25)

Age
0.00
(0.01)

Married
0.53
(0.24)
*
Education
-0.42
(0.27)

Income
-0.02
(0.02)

Religious Attitudes
0.50
(0.26)

Authoritarianism
0.14
(0.09)

Political Knowledge
-0.02
(0.12)

Black
-1.85
(0.66)
**
Hispanic
-1.75
(0.45)
***
Asian
-1.37
(0.66)
*
Other Races
-0.78
(0.38)
*
Economic Status
0.84
(0.13)
***
Black Thermometer
0.00
(0.01)

Uni Affirmative Action
0.16
(0.08)

Black Welfare
0.14
(0.09)

Affirmative Action
0.27
(0.38)

Attitudes on Slavery
0.26
(0.10)
*
Black Work Ethic
-0.24
(0.12)
*
Constant
-6.52
(1.41)

Pseudo R-Squared
0.77


Observations 1,171
*p<.05


**P<.01



***p<.001









Trump Vote




Odds Ratio
Robust Standard Errors





Republican
4.84
(1.72)
***
Democrat
0.11
(0.04)
***
Ideology
1.75
(0.23)
***
Female
0.83
(0.21)

Age
1.00
(0.01)

Married
1.70
(0.42)
*
Education
0.66
(0.18)

Income
0.98
(0.02)

Religious Attitudes
1.65
(0.43)

Authoritarianism
1.15
(0.11)

Political Knowledge
0.98
(0.12)

Black
0.16
(0.10)
**
Hispanic
0.17
(0.08)
***
Asian
0.25
(0.17)
*
Other Races
0.46
(0.18)
*
Economic Status
2.31
(0.31)
***
Black Thermometer
1.00
(0.01)

Uni Affirmative Action
1.17
(0.09)

Black Welfare
1.15
(0.10)

Affirmative Action
1.31
(0.50)

Attitudes on Slavery
1.29
(0.13)
*
Black Work Ethic
0.79
(0.09)
*
Constant
0.00
(0.00)

Pseudo R-Squared
0.77


Observations 1,171
*p<.05



**P<.01



***p<.001




            The McFadden pseudo-r squared result of .77 shows that the independent variables explain 77 percent of the variation in the respondent vote choice. As expected, the strongest independent variable is party identification as Republicans were 4.84 times more likely than Independents to vote for Trump. Furthermore, a one point increase on the ideological scale towards conservativism made the respondent 1.7 times more likely to vote for Trump.
            The results also show relatively strong support for part of the blind-retrospection hypothesis. Self-reported economic status is one of the strongest independent variables. A one unit increase in the ordinal scale measure towards economic dissatisfaction made the respondent 2.31 times more likely to vote for Trump.

However, the results for political knowledge did not produce statistical significance. Furthermore, a separate regression analysis was conducted interacting economic status with political knowledge but produced results in the opposite direction as expected. While Trump voters were voting retrospectively, they were not doing so blindly. They were regularly following the news and not basing their opinion purely on their personal status.
            Furthermore, the results show much weaker support for the racism hypothesis. While ethnic minorities were far more likely to vote against Donald Trump with a high degree of statistical significance, most of the measurements of old fashioned and symbolic racism did not produce statistically significant results. All of the coefficients are in the right direction, but only the questions on attitudes towards slavery and black work ethic are statistically significant at a P-level below .05. While voters were polarized based on racial identity, the effects of symbolic racism and old fashioned racism were more limited.

            There are several limitations to this study. For one, the measurements of racism do not measure subconscious racism nor did they account for the fact that survey respondents may have not been honest in their responses to the questions on race. Furthermore, it is possible that tensions between blacks and whites were not as salient for this election like in 2008. Measurements of ethnocentrism as well as attitudes towards Muslims and Hispanic immigrants may produce more statistically significant results. Lastly, this study does not determine why opinions on personal economic status had such a strong effect on vote choice. Do these opinions match real deteriorations in the status of living for many Trump voters, or are these opinions shaped by the conservative media they are reading and watching? The lack of significance for the income variable in the regression analysis indicates that the later may be truer than the former.




Work Cited
Bartels, Achen.  2016. Democracy for Realists.  Princeton University Press: Princeton.

DeSante, Christopher D. 2013. “Working Twice as Hard to Get Half as Far: Race, Work Ethic, and America's Deserving Poor.” American Journal of Political Science, 57(2): 342-356.

Druckman, James, Erik Peterson, and Rune Slothus.  2013.  “How Elite Partisan Polarization Affects Public Opinion Formation,” American Political Science Review, 107: 57-79.

Kinder, Donald and David Sears. 1981. “Prejudice and Politics: Symbolic Racism versus Racial Threats to the Good Life," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40(3): 414-431.

Nail, Paul R., Helen C. Harton, and Brian P. Decker. 2003. "Political Orientation and Modern versus Aversive Racism: Tests of Dovidio and Gaertner's (1998) Integrated Model." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4): 754.

Scaglion, Richard and Richard Condon. 1980. “The Structure of Black and White Attitudes towards Police,” Human Organization, 39(3): 280-283.

Schlesinger, Joseph.  1985. “The New American Political Party,” American Political Science Review, 79:  1152-1169.

Sniderman, Paul M., Thomas Piazza, Philip E. Tetlock, and Ann Kendrick. 1991. “The New Racism.” American Journal of Political Science, 35(2): 423-447.

Taber, Charles, and Milton Lodge.  2006.   “Motivated Skepticism in the Evaluation of Political Beliefs,” American Journal of Political Science, 50:  755-769.

Tarman, Christopher and David Sears. 2005. “The Conceptualization and Measurement of Symbolic Racism.” The Journal of Politics, 67(3): 731–761.

Terkildsen, Nayda. 1993. “When White Voters Evaluate Black Candidates: The Processing Implications of Candidate Skin Color, Prejudice, and Self-Monitoring.” American Journal of Political Science, 37(4): 1032-1053.

Tesler, Michael. 2013. “The Return of Old-Fashioned Racism to White Americans’ Partisan Preferences in the Early Obama Era,” The Journal of Politics, 75(1): 110-123.

“User’s Guide and Codebook for the ANES 2016 Time Series Study,” http://www.electionstudies.org/studypages/anes_timeseries_2016/anes_timeseries_2016_userguidecodebook.pdf, 2016.

Weizer, Ronald. 2004. “Race and Perceptions of Police Misconduct,” Social Problems, 51(3): 305-25.
Zaller, John. 1992. The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.



[1] “User’s Guide and Codebook for the ANES 2016 Time Series Study,” http://www.electionstudies.org/studypages/anes_timeseries_2016/anes_timeseries_2016_userguidecodebook.pdf, 2016.